Study challenges evolution of how humans acquired language

0
984

A GENE implicated in affect­ing speech and language, FOXP2, is held up as a “textbook” example of posi­tive selection on a human-specific trait. But in a paper in the journal Cell on Aug 2, researchers challenge this finding.

Their analysis of genetic data from a diverse sample of modern people and Neanderthals saw no evidence for recent, human-specific selection of FOXP2 and revises the history of how we think humans acquired language.

A paper published in 2002 claimed there was a selective sweep rela­tively recently in human evolutionary history that could largely account for our linguistic abilities and even help explain how modern humans were able to flourish so rapidly in Africa within the last 50-100,000 years, said senior author Brenna Henn, a population geneticist at Stony Brook University and UC Davis.

Henn was immediately interested in the dating of these mutations and the selective sweep. She wanted to re-analyse FOXP2 with larger and more diverse data sets, especially in more African populations.

More genetic information available now

Henn says that when the original 2002 work was done, the research­ers did not have access to the modern sequencing technology that now provides data on whole genomes, so they only analysed a small fraction of the FOXP2 gene in about 20 individuals, mostly of Eur­asian descent.

“We wanted to test whether their hypothesis stood up against a larger, more diverse data set that more explicitly controlled for human demography,” she said.

FOXP2 is highly expressed during brain development and regulates some muscle movements aiding in language production. When the gene isn’t expressed, it causes a condition called specific language impairment in which people may perform normally on cognitive tests but cannot produce spoken language. FOXP2 has also been shown to regulate language-like behaviours in mice and songbirds.

“In the past five years, several archaic hominin genomes have been sequenced, and FOXP2 was among the first genes examined because it was so important and supposedly human-specific,” said first author Elizabeth Atkinson of Stony Brook University and the Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT. “But this new data threw a wrench in the 2002 paper’s timeline, and it turns out that the FOXP2 mutations we thought to be human-specific, aren’t.”

Atkinson and her colleagues assembled mostly publicly available data from diverse human genomes – both modern and archaic – and analysed the entire FOXP2 gene while comparing it to the sur­rounding genetic information to better understand the context for its evolution.

Despite attempting a series of different statistical tests, they were unable to replicate this idea that there was any positive selection occurring for FOXP2.

The researchers hope that this paper will serve as a template for other population geneticists to conduct similar projects on human evolutionary history in the future.

“We’re interested in figuring out, on a genetic level, what makes us human,” Henn said.

“This paper shows how important it is to use a diverse set of humans in studying the evolution of all of us as a species.

There’s a severe Eurocentric bias in a lot of medical and other scientific studies, but we’ve found a scientific impetus for emphasis­ing diversity and inclusivity in data collection because it clearly yields more accurate results.”